The Paradox of Choice

What is the Paradox of Choice?

The paradox of choice is a concept introduced by psychologist Barry Schwartz which suggests that the more options we have, the less satisfied we feel with our decision. This phenomenon occurs because having too many choices requires more cognitive effort, leading to decision fatigue and increased regret over our choices.

A cartoon titled "Paradox of Choice" shows a stick figure on the left saying, "This is a tough one..." while looking at a shelf labeled "Milks" with various cartons. The milk options include "Skim Milk," "2% Milk," "3.125% Milk," "15% Peanut Milk," "Milk: 100% Non-Human," and "Milk from Larry" with an image of a cow.

The Basic Idea

Imagine that you need milk, so you go to the grocery store to pick some up. When you get to the dairy aisle, you see that there are dozens of options. These days, not only do you have to make a decision on the percentage of fat you want (1%, 2%, skim, etc.), but also what source you want your milk to be coming from: cows, almonds, soybeans, oats…the list goes on. Almost dumbfounded, you stand in front of the aisle and have no idea what milk to pick. There are so many choices that you are overwhelmed.

This phenomenon is known as the paradox of choice, and it is becoming a concern in the modern world, where more and more options are becoming easily available to us. If we only had to choose between 1% and 2% milk, it would be easier to know which option we prefer since we can easily weigh the pros and cons. When the number of choices increases, so does the difficulty of knowing what is best. Instead of increasing our freedom to have what we want, the paradox of choice suggests that having too many choices actually limits our freedom.

Learning to choose is hard. Learning to choose well is harder. And learning to choose well in a world of unlimited possibilities is harder still, perhaps too hard.


– Barry Schwartz in his book The Paradox of Choice1

Key Terms

Choice Overload: The tendency for people to get overwhelmed when they are presented with a large number of options, often used interchangeably with the term paradox of choice.

Maximizer: An individual who seeks out the most optimal (maximum utility) outcome when making a decision.2

Satisficer: an individual who is more concerned about making a decision that is ‘good enough’ and fulfills their desired criteria instead of making the best decision.2

Choice Architecture: Techniques that are implemented to organize the context under which people make decisions in order to influence them to make certain decisions.3 For example, changing how choices are presented to consumers could help reduce overwhelm without infringing on their individual freedom.

Second-Order Decisions: Decisions people make to simplify future decisions. These initial choices act as a strategy to make ordinary decision-making easier or simpler, like establishing a regular breakfast routine so you don’t have to decide what to eat every morning.4

Opportunity Cost/Missed Opportunities: The costs or benefits of options that are not chosen when you make a certain choice. Choosing one option often means sacrificing another, but hypothesizing about missed opportunities can be mentally costly to calculate when facing an overwhelming number of choices.5

History

The paradox of choice was popularized by American psychologist Barry Schwartz when he published his book, The Paradox of Choice: Why More is Less, in 2004. Schwartz, who has long studied the ways in which economics and psychology intersect, became interested in seeing the way that choices were affecting the happiness of citizens in Western societies. He identified that the range of choices that we have available to us these days is far greater than people had in the past; however, consumer satisfaction has not increased as much as traditional economics theories might expect.6

One of the central tenets of western societies, especially America, is freedom. This freedom is often associated with choice, with a belief that greater choice is equated to greater freedom. This logic is easy to follow: instead of being forced to choose between one or two different options, people have the freedom to choose between an almost unlimited number of options. Businesses and corporations often also follow this ideology, believing that more choices will lead to greater customer utility.7

However, Schwartz found that having this unparalleled plethora of choice in the modern world was actually causing people to be less happy with their decisions. He found that instead of increasing decision satisfaction, having too many options made people less likely to be satisfied that they had made the best decision. While freedom is important, Schwartz explains that there is a fine line between having the freedom to choose what you want and being paralyzed in the face of too many options. In his paper “Doing Better but Feeling Worse” that came out in the same year as his book, Schwartz and co-author Andrew Ward claimed that “unconstrained freedom leads to paralysis.” (1)5

The Original Paradox of Choice Study

The study that initially sparked Barry Schwartz’s interest in the matter was conducted by Sheena Iyengar and Mark Lepper—also important figures in behavioral science—in 2001.10 In their paper “When Choice is Demotivating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing”, Iyengar and Lepper outlined an experiment they conducted where too many options caused consumers to be less likely to buy a product, although they did not coin this phenomenon as the paradox of choice.10

In their study, Iyengar and Lepper wanted to see how the volume of choice impacted consumer behavior. In the first variable, shoppers at a grocery store encountered a display table with gourmet jam. If shoppers tasted at least one jam (they were free to try as many as they liked), they were given a $1 discount coupon to use to purchase any jam. In the extensive-choice condition, the display table had 24 different varieties of gourmet jam. In the limited-choice condition, the display table had only six different varieties of gourmet jam.

stick figure tasting jam illustrating the paradox of choice

Iyengar and Lepper measured both the number of individuals in each condition that visited the display table and tried jams and how many consumers in each condition actually made a purchase. The psychologists found that while more shoppers that passed the display table with 24 jams stopped to try when compared to the number of individuals who stopped at the display table with six jams, people in the limited-choice condition were actually more likely to make a purchase. The researchers concluded that while an abundance of options might initially seem attractive to consumers, having too many options might actually cause someone not to make any decision at all.10

People

Barry Schwartz

Schwartz is a Professor of Social Theory and Social Action at Swarthmore College who popularized the concept of the paradox of choice. In his book, The Paradox of Choice: Why More is Less, he explores the profound challenges people face when confronting overwhelming options and offers practical steps to help readers limit their choices and derive greater satisfaction from their decisions.5 Schwartz is known for critiquing the rational economic model of human behavior—which assumes that more choice leads to better outcomes, overlooking the psychological complexities of human decision-making.

Sheena Iyengar

Expert on choice and best-selling author of The Art of Choosing, Iyengar has significantly contributed to studying human decision-making.13 Her research on choice regularly appears in books and major news outlets such as the New York Times, Wall Street Journal, and The Economist, and she has received several awards for her work. Iyengar is the S.T. Lee Professor of Business in the Management Division at Columbia Business School, where she teaches courses on leadership, decision-making, and behavioral economics.

behavior change 101

Start your behavior change journey at the right place

Consequences

A graph illustrating the relationship between the "No. of Choices" on the x-axis and "Subjective State" on the y-axis, which ranges from "Negative" to "Positive." The curve rises, peaks at a "Sweet Spot," and then declines, showing that after a certain number of choices, the subjective state becomes less positive and eventually negative.

The paradox of choice can pop up any time we’re confronted with options, influencing all sorts of everyday decisions, from ordering a cup of coffee to picking out a pair of jeans. Facing overwhelming choices—even when the decision is trivial—can have several consequences for our mental well-being.

Anxiety and Decision Fatigue

As we make social, scientific and technological advances, we find ourselves with more options than were available to previous generations. The choice of what milk to buy is but one example of the ways in which we have become privy to an abundance of choices. There are hundreds of options for what kind of clothes we should purchase, the groceries we should buy, the car we should be driving, the beauty products we should be using, the restaurant we should eat at… the list goes on and on. While on the surface, the profusion of options might seem like it should increase consumer satisfaction, since people are more likely to find one option that fits their particular wants and needs, we can also become very overwhelmed. While it is easy to choose option A if there is only an option B, it becomes much harder to gauge the value and utility of A when there are options A-Z. As a result, we encounter a choice overload, which contributes to anxiety and decision fatigue.

Decision fatigue occurs when our limited stamina for making decisions is drained. Not only does this result in mental exhaustion, but it can also negatively impact our self-control. For example, one study found that doctors were more likely to prescribe unnecessary antibiotics after working for several hours, and another found that parole judges were more likely to grant parole in the morning than at the end of the workday (after making numerous decisions).14 Researchers Polman and Vohs suggest this is because decision fatigue reduces our self-regulatory resources, which are necessary for making ethical decisions. 

This happens to everyday consumers as well: those facing decision fatigue often opt for easier, more impulsive decisions, which might mean splurging on more expensive options that are prominently displayed or recommended by salespeople. For instance, people buying cars tend to choose more default features at the end of the buying process, suggesting that they have exhausted their decision-making capacity after selecting among multiple options earlier in the process.14 Have you ever grabbed takeout on the way home from a large grocery trip because you’re too mentally exhausted to decide what to make for dinner? Decision fatigue can lead us toward more convenient choices like these, even if they aren’t in our best interest.

Too Many Choices, Not Enough Time

The paradox of choice is not only a concern for economics and consumer satisfaction but an issue that is popping up in various areas of our lives as our possibilities come nearer and nearer to being endless. Moreover, the internet and social media has made it easier for us to see all the different options that are available to us, no longer having to physically stand in a store to determine what our options are. Fast-paced advances being made in technology and science also mean that there seems to be new kinds of jobs created on the daily—not to mention all the different social media applications that created a whole host of jobs (influencers, social media specialists, etc). Social media has also changed the way that we choose a life partner. For example, dating apps like Tinder and Hinge have enabled people to have dozens of options of who to date at their fingertips (more on this later).

One clear consequence of this explosion in choice is the significant time required to deliberate over all our options. How much time do you spend researching and reading reviews before ordering something online? How long does it take for you to decide what to order for dinner, where to go on vacation, or what movie to stream on Netflix? Choosing among the sheer number of options at your fingertips can be incredibly time-consuming—so much so that you might run out of time and miss out on making a choice altogether. Have you ever spent so long deciding what to do on Friday night that you just stayed home? When we spend too much time considering hypothetical trade-offs, we miss out on opportunities to enjoy spontaneous moments or try something new, instead defaulting to our regular routines like streaming the same old shows or ordering from the same takeout place.

Post-Choice Regret

Following Herbert Simon’s ideas on bounded rationality and satisficing, Schwartz identified that the paradox of choice carries the most consequence for people that are maximizers. Maximizers, unlike satisficers,  have a kind of obsession with choice optimization—they are concerned with making the best choice instead of simply making a choice that they are happy with. When there are many options available to maximizers, it becomes harder for them to determine which is the best, which can cause them to feel a great deal of regret after they have made a choice. Additionally, when there are more options, opportunity costs are greater and can leave people with more regret.

Instead of adhering to the belief that freedom of choice is unbounded, Schwartz suggests that “self-determination within significant constraints – within ‘rules’ of some sort – that leads to well-being, to optimal functioning” (1).5 It is within this ideology that Schwartz advocates that the role of psychology and behavioral economics is to find the kind of limitations on freedom that can lead to the greatest level of happiness within society.

Controversies

An illustration of a stick figure standing with hands on their head, surrounded by a chaotic swirl of lines and arrows labeled with words such as "Yes," "No," "Maybe," "Never," "I'm Stuck," "Certainly," and "Lost." The image conveys a sense of confusion and indecision.

Are fewer choices always better? Perhaps not. The paradox of choice assumes that having fewer options helps us make decisions with less anxiety and greater satisfaction, but evidence suggests that people actually prefer having more options in certain contexts.

People Like Having Options

While many studies have demonstrated that people are less satisfied with the decisions they make the more options are available, other studies have conflicting evidence. For example, the decoy effect suggests that we feel more strongly about an option when there are three options than if there are only two. The paradox of choice has been criticized for not having enough concrete and scientific evidence behind it and critics often offer up countering evidence, such as the fact that Starbucks, which boasts a menu with hundreds of possibilities and customizations, is an incredibly popular and profitable company.8 Another phenomenon that counters the paradox of choice is single-choice aversion, identified by Daniel Mochon, professor of marketing. Single-choice aversion suggests that people are unwilling to choose an attractive option if there are no alternative options since they have nothing to compare it against.8

The Sweet Spot Between Choice and Overwhelm

Barry Schwartz, the father of the paradox of choice, acknowledges that these controversial findings are likely apparent. He suggests that if all the studies based on the ways that options impact choice were compiled, we would likely find that they average out; sometimes more options leads to increased satisfaction, sometimes it leads to diminished satisfaction. However, instead of this opposing evidence suggesting that we don’t need to concern ourselves over the impact of choice, Schwartz suggests that it is about finding the right balance between having too many options and not enough options. He doesn’t think the studies that offer results different to what is expected by the paradox of choice undermine the effect’s credibility; instead, research needs to become more nuanced to find the magic number that can optimize people’s happiness.9

Choice Architecture: Nudging Over Restriction

Clearly, people appreciate the freedom to make their own choices. So how do you help people make better decisions in a world of overwhelming abundance without limiting their autonomy? Choice architecture and nudging strategies may be a better solution than restricting people’s choices—and can be surprisingly effective at directing people toward certain decisions. For example, a large two-year study on choice architecture found that simply increasing the visibility and accessibility of healthy food options in a cafeteria led to an increase in healthy food sales and a decrease in unhealthy food sales.15

Rather than reducing the number of choices available to people, choice architecture works by subtly changing how options are presented, nudging people toward options that are in their best interest. Helping people navigate overwhelming choices more easily can be as simple as organizing their options into categories or highlighting desirable options. Consider how Netflix curates recommendations based on your watch history to help you discover content without having to scroll endlessly through every movie on the platform. Would you rather Netflix just remove some content to make your choice easier? Probably not. We can avoid the ethical issues of restricting people’s freedom of choice—and their individual autonomy—by presenting options more manageably rather than removing them altogether.

Case Studies

Tinder and the Paradox of Choice

An illustration of a stick figure holding two smartphones, each displaying a profile. The left phone shows "Tony, 23" with an image of a smiling stick figure and the right phone shows "John, 21" with another stick figure. Both profiles have the option to accept (green checkmark) or reject (red X) the profile, suggesting a choice between the two. The background is divided into purple and orange, matching the colors of the phones.

Back in the day, our grandparents had a limited pool of options for who they could date. Without the internet, they had to rely on meeting people in-person and the number of single people they met within a suitable age-range was not very large.

These days, dating apps like Tinder have changed the game. We no longer have to rely on meeting someone that runs in the same circles as we do. The app allows us to swipe through potential matches that we may never have crossed paths with, who are essentially complete strangers. At first thought, this seems great – we now have access to all these people who we never would have been able to conventionally meet!

However, with so many possibilities for a partner right at your fingertips, so too enters the paradox of choice. How do you pick one person to date when there are so many other options out there that might be better? This might mean that you make rash decisions since you don’t have enough time to look through all the options or you might careless with your decisions and swipe right on everyone.11

As a result of the paradox of choice, people also seem less likely to commit or spend the necessary quality time getting to know someone since they can just get right back on the app. One user, in an article for Stanford Daily, writes “the seemingly infinite supply of options allowed me to care less, to distance myself, to treat people like items in an online shopping cart… as a result, I found myself deeply unhappy with all of it”.12

The Paradox of Choice and 401(k) Participation

The paradox effect may also influence whether or not people make contributions to 401(k) retirement plans offered by their employers. Most 401(k) plans offer employees countless investment opportunities, and this wide array of options seems like it should be beneficial for account holders. You would think that providing more fund options—so employees can have greater freedom to pick their investments—would drive employee interest. However, a 2004 study by Sheena Iyengar, Wei Jiang, and Gur Huberman suggests that extensive choice can actually reduce people’s willingness to participate in these retirement plans.16

Analyzing data from nearly 800,000 employee records, Iyengar and colleagues found that the likelihood of employee participation decreased when 401(k) plans offered more funds. In fact, every additional 10 funds reduced participation by up to 2%. Plans that offered only two funds had a peak participation rate of 75%, but plans with 59 funds had participation rates as low as 60%. Out of all the plans examined in the study, those offering less than 10 funds had significantly higher participation rates than those offering between more than 10 options. 

The researchers suggest that providing employees with generous 401(k) options may actually intimidate them rather than entice them to invest in their personal retirement plans. This study highlights the challenges that the paradox of choice creates when trying to engage people in complex financial decision-making, indicating that people might require additional guidance or recommendations to help them navigate their choices and feel confident about making financial decisions.

Related TDL Content

Why More Choice Means Less Freedom

Opening with a joke where an individual enters a cafe and faces option after option of how they would like their tea (what kind, what kind of milk they want with it, what kind of sweetener they’d like, etc.), our writer Arash Sharma explores the risks of an abundance of choice. After outlining the problems caused by having too many options, Sharma offers up some advice to both consumers and producers on how to limit the dissatisfaction that accompanies the paradox of choice.

The Behavioral Science Guide to Gift Giving

In this article, our writer Preeti Kotamarthi examines the ways in which behavioral science can help people make the best decisions when it comes to giving gifts, an important social custom. Kotarmarthi understands that there are so many choices with what to buy our friends and family that we often fall victim to the paradox of choice and make suboptimal decisions. Kotarmarthi offers up strategies to reduce the effect that the paradox of choice has on our happiness and the happiness of the gift receivers.

Do I Agree? Cognitive Bias and Terms of Service

Describing people’s tendency to scroll through terms of service and click agree without actually reading them, our writer Tiago Rodrigo examines the cognitive bias behind our disregard of the specificities of terms of service. He suggests that one reason behind this tendency is the fact that websites and services overload us with information we become overwhelmed and make a rash decision without taking the time to consider the details and nuances of our decision. The overload of information parallels the paradox of choice and can cause consumers to feel a great deal of anxiety and distress.

Sources

  1. Goodreads. (n.d.). Barry Schwartz (Author of the paradox of choice) Quotes. Retrieved January 6, 2021, from https://www.goodreads.com/author/quotes/6957.Barry_Schwartz
  2. Bernstein, E. (2014, October 6). How You Make Decisions Says a Lot About How Happy You Are. Wall Street Journal. https://www.wsj.com/articles/how-you-make-decisions-says-a-lot-about-how-happy-you-are-1412614997
  3. The Decision Lab. (2020, December 15). Nudges.https://thedecisionlab.com/reference-guide/economics/nudges
  4. Sunstein, C. R., & Ullmann-Margalit, E. (1998). Second-order decisions. SSRN Electronic Journalhttps://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.101268
  5. Schwartz, B., & Ward, A. (2012). Doing better but feeling worse: The paradox of choice. Positive Psychology in Practice, 86-104. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939338.ch6
  6. Barry Schwartz: Are we happier when we have more options? (2013, November 15). National Public Radio. https://www.npr.org/2013/11/15/245034685/are-we-happier-when-we-have-more-options
  7. Barry Schwartz: Are we happier when we have more options? (2013, November 15). National Public Radio. https://www.npr.org/2013/11/15/245034685/are-we-happier-when-we-have-more-options
  8. Thompson, D. (2013, August 19). More Is More: Why the Paradox of Choice Might Be a Myth. The Atlantic. https://www.theatlantic.com/business/archive/2013/08/more-is-more-why-the-paradox-of-choice-might-be-a-myth/278658/
  9. Schwartz, B. (2014, January 29). Is the famous ‘paradox of choice’ a myth? PBS NewsHour. https://www.pbs.org/newshour/economy/is-the-famous-paradox-of-choice
  10. Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2001). When Choice is Demotivating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology79(6), 995-1006. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511618031.017
  11. Nixon, C. (2020, October 7). Do Dating Apps Affect Relationship Decision Making? The Decision Lab. https://thedecisionlab.com/insights/society/do-dating-apps-affect-relationship-decision-making/
  12. Bersh, L. (2020, February 26). On the paradox of choice, Tinder. The Stanford Daily. https://www.stanforddaily.com/2020/02/26/on-the-paradox-of-choice-tinder/
  13. Iyengar, S. (n.d.). Bio. Sheena Iyengar. https://sheenaiyengar.com/bio/ 
  14. Polman, E., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Decision Fatigue, Choosing for Others, and Self-Construal. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(5), 471-478. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616639648
  15. Thorndike, A. N., Riis, J., Sonnenberg, L. M., & Levy, D. E. (2014). Traffic-Light Labels and Choice Architecture: Promoting Healthy Food Choices. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 46(2), 143-149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2013.10.002 
  16. Iyengar, S., Huberman, G., & Jiang, G. (2004). How Much Choice is Too Much? Contributions to 401(k) Retirement Plans. In O. S. Mitchell & S. P. Utkus (Eds.), Pension Design and Structure: New Lessons from Behavioral Finance. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/0199273391.003.0005

About the Authors

A man in a blue, striped shirt smiles while standing indoors, surrounded by green plants and modern office decor.

Dan Pilat

Dan is a Co-Founder and Managing Director at The Decision Lab. He is a bestselling author of Intention - a book he wrote with Wiley on the mindful application of behavioral science in organizations. Dan has a background in organizational decision making, with a BComm in Decision & Information Systems from McGill University. He has worked on enterprise-level behavioral architecture at TD Securities and BMO Capital Markets, where he advised management on the implementation of systems processing billions of dollars per week. Driven by an appetite for the latest in technology, Dan created a course on business intelligence and lectured at McGill University, and has applied behavioral science to topics such as augmented and virtual reality.

A smiling man stands in an office, wearing a dark blazer and black shirt, with plants and glass-walled rooms in the background.

Dr. Sekoul Krastev

Sekoul is a Co-Founder and Managing Director at The Decision Lab. He is a bestselling author of Intention - a book he wrote with Wiley on the mindful application of behavioral science in organizations. A decision scientist with a PhD in Decision Neuroscience from McGill University, Sekoul's work has been featured in peer-reviewed journals and has been presented at conferences around the world. Sekoul previously advised management on innovation and engagement strategy at The Boston Consulting Group as well as on online media strategy at Google. He has a deep interest in the applications of behavioral science to new technology and has published on these topics in places such as the Huffington Post and Strategy & Business.

About us

We are the leading applied research & innovation consultancy

Our insights are leveraged by the most ambitious organizations

Image

I was blown away with their application and translation of behavioral science into practice. They took a very complex ecosystem and created a series of interventions using an innovative mix of the latest research and creative client co-creation. I was so impressed at the final product they created, which was hugely comprehensive despite the large scope of the client being of the world's most far-reaching and best known consumer brands. I'm excited to see what we can create together in the future.

Heather McKee

BEHAVIORAL SCIENTIST

GLOBAL COFFEEHOUSE CHAIN PROJECT

OUR CLIENT SUCCESS

$0M

Annual Revenue Increase

By launching a behavioral science practice at the core of the organization, we helped one of the largest insurers in North America realize $30M increase in annual revenue.

0%

Increase in Monthly Users

By redesigning North America's first national digital platform for mental health, we achieved a 52% lift in monthly users and an 83% improvement on clinical assessment.

0%

Reduction In Design Time

By designing a new process and getting buy-in from the C-Suite team, we helped one of the largest smartphone manufacturers in the world reduce software design time by 75%.

0%

Reduction in Client Drop-Off

By implementing targeted nudges based on proactive interventions, we reduced drop-off rates for 450,000 clients belonging to USA's oldest debt consolidation organizations by 46%

Read Next

Notes illustration

Eager to learn about how behavioral science can help your organization?