Do Dating Apps Affect Relationship Decision Making?

read time - icon

0 min read

Oct 03, 2018

"‘Good relationships are not born out of complex algorithms, because attraction is unpredictable…’ (Joel, Eastwick, and Finkel 2017)"

Times are changing, people are becoming more tech savvy and are living fast paced and busy lives. Increased work hours and more demanding responsibilities often impedes on our ability to socialise, consequentially creating a negative impact on personal life. One such impediment that is becoming more common is the ability to seek a potential relationship or life partner.

Evidence of this emerging difficulty can be seen with the boom of online dating smartphone apps such as Tinder, Badoo, and Plenty of fish. Such apps seek to resolve this growing disparity between work and social life, allowing the individual to scour over potential matches whilst on their commute, at their desk, or on their sofa.

A survey conducted by Statista (2017) showed that these three platforms rank in the top 4 alongside match.com, where regular respondent usage ranged between 32 – 45% of singles. With increased popularity, and reduced stigma, around their use – online dating apps have fundamentally changed the dating landscape. However, change can often bring about new risks.

The Risks of Virtual Dating 

Creating a culture of short-term relationships that never truly materialise may subsequently have a negative effect on well-being and mental health, especially as 1 in 6 individuals reportedly develop a mental health problem such as anxiety over their lives (Stansfeld et al 2016). Such increases in anxiety may arise from concerns of self-esteem that come under fire from poor quality conversations, dates, and relationships that create doubts of self-image. Considering how issues such as these are hastened by dating apps, it is necessary to ask are dating apps improve relationships, and if not, how can they be improved?

Behavioral science is well equipped to explore this domain through the collaboration of economics, psychology, and sociology to understand individuals dating choices and behaviors.  Despite many longstanding clichés of love being a function of the heart, it is now widely accepted and observed to be a function of the brain (Bartels and Zeki 2000; Zeki 2007).

Individuals consider an array of multiple factors that make the perfect romantic match, such as their personality, hobbies, interests, and physical aspects to name a few. These aspects therefore lend themselves to a series of biases and heuristics that influence decision making, and ultimately may produce romantic outcomes that create imperfect or even negative relationships.

For instance, behavioral science explores the role of visceral factors – such as love – on decision making, showing how these temporary states of arousal lend themselves to behaviors which deviate from individuals stated preferences. This was famously shown by Ariely and Loewenstein (2006), who through a series of experiments on male students, showed that ‘sexual arousal has a strong impact on all three areas of judgement and decision making’ characterized as the heat of the moment effect (Ariely and Loewenstein, 2006).

By understanding the mechanisms of such cognitive barriers, behavioral science is perfectly suited to express not only why these decisions are made, but how these can be overcome with potential interventions. The amalgamation of economic decision making, psychological states of emotions, and sociological factors of relationships allows for the mixture of rivalling practices to be combined in a multidisciplinary and scientific way.

In doing so, behavioral science can seek to develop novel and unique insights into how love and emotions play a role in our lives and the current dating climate.

Plenty of Fish, or Too Many?

So, what are the behavioral mechanisms behind the use of dating apps? And how can they induce negative emotional outcomes? One behavioral tendency considers the ease and convenience dating platforms offer and in particular, the sheer volume of information presented when making choices of potential partners, seen with Tinder and Badoo respectively receiving 57 million U.K users in 2017 (Belton, 2018).

This concept is called the paradox of choice, where an increased freedom of choice – in this case, choice of people – results in decreased subjective well-being (Schwartz 2004). This paradox has been witnessed when individuals are choosing between types of jam. When given the choice of either 24 or 6 kinds of jam, there was a significant reduction in purchases by respondents presented with 24 compared to 6 (Iynegar and Lepper 2000).

Evidence from Schawrtz (2004) and Iyneger and Lepper (2000) shows that this paradox occurs due to inherent difficulties humans have in managing complex choices. Increasing the number of attractive alternatives – such as picking an alternative, deferring the option, choosing the default or opting out – has been shown to increase the level of internal conflict in decision making (Shafir, Simonsen and Tversky, 1993). Furthermore, the behavioral tendency of narrow framing exacerbates this difficulty, meaning that when more alternatives are presented, individuals tend to use a rule of thumb based on a small sample of all alternatives (Hauser and Wernerfelt, 1990).

While experimenting with jams can be considered somewhat crude, the paradox can be applied to dating apps. The sheer volume facilitates the tendency to increase the likelihood of objectification and ill-advised decisions (Finkel et al. 2012), allocating a preference for rushed choices in light of a mass of potential candidates. This can be seen with individuals potentially swiping right for all candidates, leading to choices being made without considerable thought or none at all.

With this notion, the user may seem confused to why they have been matched with certain individuals, due to a lack of consideration when swiping through individuals in such a hasty manner and looking at individuals on face value.

Is Desire Feasible?

In line with a focusing on skin deep features, a second behavioral principle involved in dating app decision making is the concept of construal level theory (Liberman and Trope 1998). Construal level theory (CLT) defined as ‘an account of how psychological distance influences individual thoughts and behavior’ (Trope and Liberman 2010) where objects and contexts are interpreted as either being low or high level.

A low level of construal provides focus on the core details of an object or context, such as the color, temperature, or size. In contrast, a high level of construal takes focus on overarching perceptions, and essentially differ between looking at objective details or the bigger picture.

By exploring  the foundations of CLT, it has been shown that levels of construal are affected through different domains of psychological distance – such as time, space, social, and hypothetical – that alter individual perception and factors associated with decision making (Wakslak, Liberman, and Trope 2006; Malkoc, Zauberman, and Bettman 2010). In relation to dating apps, the use of a computer-mediated-communication platform (Finkel et al. 2012) on a smartphone creates an increase in spatial and social distance, and therefore a higher level of construal.

Additionally, different weightings are given to different objects depending on the level of psychological distance concerning their attributes. Through a series of 5 different choice experiments targeting pre, intra, and post decision making, Lu, Xie, and Xu (2012) found that concerns of desirability receive a greater weighting over more feasible attributes as psychological distance increases, consistent with past research into CLT (Todorov, Green, and Trope 2007).

This impression highlights that when individuals make choices on dating platforms – with greater psychological distance – more desirable features such as looks and physical attributes are emphasized over their feasible counterparts including personality and other deeper individual differences. Consequentially, this may lead to choices being made based on incomplete evidence of the whole individual, potentially leading to sub-optimal outcomes such as regret after a date, contributing to future communications or long-term intimacies breaking down.

Discussion

By discussing two potential behavioral mechanisms that play a role in emotional decision making, what can be done to try and mitigate these biases? One recommendation worthy of exploration would be to integrate methods of improving the level of information given to users.

The concept of salience is widely used in the world of behavioral science (Behavioral Insights Team. 2014), and could be applied to this domain by utilizing personality and compatibility tests. With this notion, Piasecki and Hanna (2011) propose an alteration to defining the paradox of choice to a lack of meaningful choices instead of the volume of choices leading to negative outcomes.

Providing a salient personality or compatibly score may allow for some potential matches to be more meaningful due to the initial perception that the two users are well suited to each other, allowing users to better allocate their time to candidates more likely to produce positive emotional outcomes, filtering down the pool of choice, and the paradox. Likewise, looking at matches on a personality / compatibility basis lends the user to be considering more feasible factors over the desirable, potentially altering behavior through different levels of construal.

Taking this idea further, it has recently been announced that the dating platform Badoo is set to scrap the mainstream swipe-interface for the use of a live stream feature, called Badoo Live (Lomas, 2018). Based on a survey of 5,000 users aged 18 – 30, Badoo found that the widely common swiping interface and use of photos lacks the “real” experience that is ascertained from a real-life scenarios (Peat, 2018).

By adding these features, Badoo has taken the first step into overcoming the current barriers to positive emotional outcomes on dating apps. The use of the live stream feature reduces the psychological distance of matches with the face-to-face communication, providing a better platform for meaningful and genuine conversations that are not over a series of texts.

The AI Governance Challenge book
eBook

The AI Governance Challenge

Conclusion

In conclusion, despite being highly convenient, dating apps can easily result in ill-advised romantic decisions due to a cognitive overload of options and abstract thinking which produce choice inconsistencies between a screen and reality. Despite concerns being raised of the apps impact on mental well-being, time is a finite resource, and these dating apps can provide a solid platform for meeting new people in a world where loneliness is a pressing social concern.

As seen with the recent innovations from the platform Badoo, changes are being made to try and replicate face-to-face meetings of the past. Ultimately, one may expect these technological advances to give rise for a virtual reality interface, where dates can be had in virtual space, recreating a face to face scenario on the go or in the comfort of the home.

References

Aretz, Wera, Inge Demuth, Kathrin Schmidt, and Jennifer Vierlein. 2010. “Partner Search in the Digital Age. Psychological Characteristics of Online-Dating-Service-Users and its Contribution to the Explanation of Different Patterns of Utilization.” Journal of Business and Media Psychology 1 (1): 8-16.

Ariely, D. and G. Loewenstein. 2006. “The Heat of the Moment: The Effect of Sexual Arousal on Sexual Decision Making.” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 19 (2): 87-98.

Ariely, D. and G. Loewenstein. 2006. “The Heat of the Moment: The Effect of Sexual Arousal on Sexual Decision Making.” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 19 (2): 87-98.

Belton, Padraig. “Love and Dating After the Tinder Revolution.” BBC News, https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/business-42988025.

Finkel, Eli J., Paul W. Eastwick, Benjamin R. Karney, Harry T. Reis, and Susan Sprecher. 2012. “Online Dating: A Critical Analysis from the Perspective of Psychological Science.” Psychological Science in the Public Interest 13 (1): 3-66.

Hauser, John R. and Birger Wernerfelt. 1990. “An Evaluation Cost Model of Consideration Sets.” Journal of Consumer Research 16 (4): 393-408.

Iyengar, Sheena S. and Mark R. Lepper. 2000. “When Choice is Demotivating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 (6): 995.

Joel, Samantha, Paul W. Eastwick, and Eli J. Finkel. 2017. “Is Romantic Desire Predictable? Machine Learning Applied to Initial Romantic Attraction.” Psychological Science 28 (10): 1478-1489.

Liberman, Nira and Yaacov Trope. 1998. “The Role of Feasibility and Desirability Considerations in Near and Distant Future Decisions: A Test of Temporal Construal Theory.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75 (1): 5.

Lomas, Natasha. “Badoo Adds Live Video Chat to its Dating Apps.” Tech Crunch, https://techcrunch.com/2018/05/02/badoo-adds-live-video-chat-to-its-dating-apps/?guccounter=1.

Lu, Jingyi, Xiaofei Xie, and Jingzhe Xu. 2013. “Desirability Or Feasibility: Self–other Decision-Making Differences.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 39 (2): 144-155.

Malkoc, Selin A., Gal Zauberman, and James R. Bettman. 2010. “Unstuck from the Concrete: Carryover Effects of Abstract Mindsets in Intertemporal Preferences.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 113 (2): 112-126.

Peat, Jack. “Dating App Badoo Prepares to Ditch the Swipe Feature After Users Label it ‘Shallow’.” Mirror, https://www.mirror.co.uk/news/uk-news/dating-app-badoo-prepares-ditch-12669344.

Piasecki, Michal and Sean Hanna. 2011. “A Redefinition of the Paradox of Choice.” In Design Computing and Cognition’10, 347-366: Springer.

Piasecki, Michal and Sean Hanna. 2011. “A Redefinition of the Paradox of Choice.” In Design Computing and Cognition’10, 347-366: Springer.

Shafir, Eldar, Itamar Simonson, and Amos Tversky. 1993. “Reason-Based Choice.” Cognition 49 (1-2): 11-36.

Stansfeld, Stephen, Charlotte Clark, Paul Bebbington, Michael King, Rachel Jenkins, and Stephen Hinchliffe. 2014. “Chapter 2: Common Mental Disorders.” Mental Health and Wellbeing in England: Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey.

Todorov, Alexander, Amir Goren, and Yaacov Trope. 2007. “Probability as a Psychological Distance: Construal and Preferences.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (3): 473-482.

Trope, Yaacov and Nira Liberman. 2010. “Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Distance.” Psychological Review 117 (2): 440.

Wakslak, CJ, S. Nussbaum, N. Liberman, and Y. Trope. 2006. “The Effect of Temporal Distance on the Structure of the Self Concept.” Unpublished Manuscript, New York University.

Zeki, Semir. 2007. “The Neurobiology of Love.” FEBS Letters 581 (14): 2575-2579.

About the Author

A man is smiling while wearing a gray and white plaid shirt, standing against a plain, light-colored background.

Charlie Nixon

University of Stirling · Behavioral Science

Charlie completed a BA (Hons) in Business Studies at Leeds Beckett University, progressing to an MSc in Behavioural Science at the University of Stirling. Charlie is now looking to start a career in the world of behavioural science application. This journey has been kickstarted by being invited to the 2018 Ogilvy Change Summer School in London, working with real clients in an research consultancy role.

About us

We are the leading applied research & innovation consultancy

Our insights are leveraged by the most ambitious organizations

Image

I was blown away with their application and translation of behavioral science into practice. They took a very complex ecosystem and created a series of interventions using an innovative mix of the latest research and creative client co-creation. I was so impressed at the final product they created, which was hugely comprehensive despite the large scope of the client being of the world's most far-reaching and best known consumer brands. I'm excited to see what we can create together in the future.

Heather McKee

BEHAVIORAL SCIENTIST

GLOBAL COFFEEHOUSE CHAIN PROJECT

OUR CLIENT SUCCESS

$0M

Annual Revenue Increase

By launching a behavioral science practice at the core of the organization, we helped one of the largest insurers in North America realize $30M increase in annual revenue.

0%

Increase in Monthly Users

By redesigning North America's first national digital platform for mental health, we achieved a 52% lift in monthly users and an 83% improvement on clinical assessment.

0%

Reduction In Design Time

By designing a new process and getting buy-in from the C-Suite team, we helped one of the largest smartphone manufacturers in the world reduce software design time by 75%.

0%

Reduction in Client Drop-Off

By implementing targeted nudges based on proactive interventions, we reduced drop-off rates for 450,000 clients belonging to USA's oldest debt consolidation organizations by 46%

Read Next

A group of people in a modern meeting room, some seated and working at a large table with laptops and cameras, while others stand and converse. The atmosphere is casual, with natural light filtering through large windows in the background.
Insight

Why Teams Make Bad Decisions

Sometimes, the best way to avoid group decision-making failures is not to make decisions as a group at all.

Notes illustration

Eager to learn about how behavioral science can help your organization?