Does Loss Aversion Impact How Judges Make Decisions?
Loss aversion is a widely documented psychological occurrence that is encapsulated in the expression “losses loom larger than gains.” [1] Basically, losing something feels worse than the corresponding pleasure derived from gaining that same thing. [2]
The phenomenon of loss aversion, which has been shown to have a basis in neuroscience, is visible across different demographics and even species, having been documented in professional financial traders, taxi drivers, young and old people, and even capuchin monkeys. [3]
Loss aversion is visible all around us in our daily lives as well, even if we don’t realize it. For example, professional golfers take more chances to avoid a loss than to obtain a commensurate gain, preferring to take more risky shots to avoid the loss of a bogey (taking one more shot than expected) than to achieve the gain of a birdie (taking one less shot than expected). [4] We see loss aversion in politics, too, with then-candidate Donald Trump using loss rhetoric frequently during his campaign for President. For example, Trump repeatedly asserted that Americans were “losing our jobs” to illegal immigrants and at one point said “[w]e’re losing everything.” [5]
But does loss aversion impact how judges make decisions in court?
The short answer is yes, probably.
For example, Rachlinski and Wistrich studied the effect of loss aversion on judges deciding employment discrimination cases. [6] Keep in mind that the law in this area doesn’t distinguish between discrimination in hiring (gain) or firing (loss)—it prohibits discrimination in both contexts. [7]
But, Rachlinski and Wistrich hypothesized that “even if all else is equal, applicants who are the victims of [hiring] discrimination face worse prospects for successfully pursuing discrimination claims than discharged employees.”
Rachlinski and Wistrich presented a hypothetical case to 146 trial judges and set up the case as follows: [8]
Frank Porter, a 61-year-old man, filed a complaint with the Florida Commission on Human Relations under the Florida Civil Rights Act of 1992, asserting that he was the victim of age discrimination at the hands of Gatorville College, a small private college.
Half of the judges received materials indicating that Porter “was one of five applicants for a resident director position at a dormitory and was not hired” and the other half received materials indicating that Porter “was one of five residential directors faced with a layoff due to an effort to reduce the number of staff—and he was let go.”
The failure to hire scenario presented at trial was as follows:
Porter was one of five applicants for four “Resident Director” positions at four dormitories at Gatorville College. All of the applicants held similar educational and professional credentials. The College chose not to hire Porter, contending that he did not have the interpersonal and staff management skills of the other applicants. Porter contends that the real reason he was not hired is because of his age. In support of this argument, he points out that the other four applicants, each of whom was hired, are all under 30, while he is 61. He also produced notes from the central administration supervisor with whom he met. Next to Porter’s name, the supervisor’s notes read, “Energy? Understanding of today’s college students?”
The termination scenario presented at trial was as follows:
Porter was one of five Resident Directors at Gatorville College, which sought to reduce its Resident Director staff from five to four. All of the Resident Directors held similar educational and professional credentials. The College chose to discharge Porter, contending that he did not have the interpersonal and staff management skills of the other applicants. Porter contends that the real reason he was discharged is because of his age. In support of this argument, he points out that the other four resident directors, each of whom was retained, are all under 30, while he is 61. He also produced notes from the central administration supervisor with whom he met. Next to Porter’s name, the supervisor’s notes read, “Energy? Understanding of today’s college students?”
The results were consistent with the authors’ initial hypothesis.
Although the legal standard was the same for both cases, how the law was applied varied based on the hiring discrimination versus firing discrimination frame. Specifically, 30% of judges who reviewed the firing discrimination version found in favor of Porter as opposed to only 14% of judges who received the hiring discrimination scenario [9]
Porter, in the firing version of events, was clearly viewed by the judges as more sympathetic, or as having been discriminated against to a worse extent, or some combination of the two.
References
[1] Daniel Kahneman & Amos Tversky, Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk, 47 Econometrica 263, 279 (1979).
[2] See Why is the pain of losing is felt twice as powerfully compared to equivalent gains? The Decision Lab, https://thedecisionlab.com/biases/loss-aversion/ (last accessed Feb. 17, 2020).
[3] See generally Peter Sokol-Hessner & Robb B. Rutledge, The Psychological and Neural Basis of Loss Aversion, 28 Current Directions in Psychol. Sci. 20 (2018); see also Michael S. Haigh & John A. List, Do Professional Traders Exhibit Myopic Loss Aversion? An Experimental Analysis, 60 J. Fin. 523 (2005) (loss aversion in financial traders); Colin Camerer, Linda Babcock, George Loewenstein, & Richard Thaler, Labor Supply of New York City Cab Drivers: One Day at a Time, 111 Q. J. Econ. 407 (1997) (loss aversion in taxi drivers); generally Robb B. Rutledge et. al., Risk Taking for Potential Reward Decreases across the Lifespan, 26 Current Biology 1634 (2016) (loss aversion in old and young); M. Keith Chen, Venkat Lakshminarayanan, & Laurie R. Santos, How Basic Are Behavioral Biases? Evidence from Capuchin Monkey Trading Behavior, 114 J. Pol. Econ. 517 (2006) (loss aversion in capuchin monkeys).
[4] Devin G. Pope & Maurice E. Schweitzer, Is Tiger Woods Loss Averse? Persistent Bias in the Face of Experience, Competition, and High Stakes, 101 Am. Econ. Rev. 129, 136 (2011) (finding loss aversion in professional golfers).
[5] James Surowiecki, Losers!, New Yorker (June 6 & 13, 2016), https:// www.newyorker.com/magazine/2016/06/06/losers-for-trump.
[6] Jeffrey J. Rachlinski & Andrew J. Wistrich, Gains, Losses, and Judges: Framing and the Judiciary, 94 Notre Dame L. Rev. 521, 573 (2018).
[7] See 42 U.S.C. § 2000e-2(a)(1) (2012) (“It shall be an unlawful employment practice for an employer to fail or refuse to hire or to discharge any individual, or otherwise to discriminate against any individual with respect to his compensation, terms, conditions, or privileges of employment, because of such individual’s race, color, religion, sex, or national origin . . . .”) (emphasis added).
[8] Rachlinski & Wistrich, Gains, Losses, and Judges, at 557.
[9] Id. at 558
[10] 9/12/05: John Roberts’ Baseball Analogy, ABC News (Sep. 12, 2005), https://abcnews.go.com/Archives/video/sept-12-2005-john-roberts-baseball-analogy-10628259.
About the Author
Tom Spiegler
Tom is a Co-Founder and Managing Director at The Decision Lab. He is interested in the intersection of decision-science and the law, with a focus on leveraging behavioral research to shape more effective public and legal policy. Tom graduated from Georgetown Law with honors. Prior to law school, Tom attended McGill University where he graduated with First Class Honors with majors in Philosophy and Psychology.
About us
We are the leading applied research & innovation consultancy
Our insights are leveraged by the most ambitious organizations
“
I was blown away with their application and translation of behavioral science into practice. They took a very complex ecosystem and created a series of interventions using an innovative mix of the latest research and creative client co-creation. I was so impressed at the final product they created, which was hugely comprehensive despite the large scope of the client being of the world's most far-reaching and best known consumer brands. I'm excited to see what we can create together in the future.
Heather McKee
BEHAVIORAL SCIENTIST
GLOBAL COFFEEHOUSE CHAIN PROJECT
OUR CLIENT SUCCESS
$0M
Annual Revenue Increase
By launching a behavioral science practice at the core of the organization, we helped one of the largest insurers in North America realize $30M increase in annual revenue.
0%
Increase in Monthly Users
By redesigning North America's first national digital platform for mental health, we achieved a 52% lift in monthly users and an 83% improvement on clinical assessment.
0%
Reduction In Design Time
By designing a new process and getting buy-in from the C-Suite team, we helped one of the largest smartphone manufacturers in the world reduce software design time by 75%.
0%
Reduction in Client Drop-Off
By implementing targeted nudges based on proactive interventions, we reduced drop-off rates for 450,000 clients belonging to USA's oldest debt consolidation organizations by 46%