Cognitive Biases Stop Us From Donating Effectively
In 2018, Americans donated $410 billion to charity,1 roughly equal to Norway’s nominal GDP that same year.2 If American charitable giving were a company, it would be one of the top three in the world in terms of revenue.3 Yet despite the overwhelming altruism of many individuals globally, over a million people die each year from preventable disease.4
Many more struggle with health issues that can be solved at relatively low costs. Take, for example, trachoma, an infectious eye disease that over 20 million people actively suffer from, many of whom live in conditions of extreme poverty.5 If left untreated, trachoma can cause an individual’s eyelids to turn inward, eventually resulting in blindness. The cost of treating trachoma with surgery has been estimated to be as low as $7.14,6 yet many still do not have access to this potentially life-changing procedure. These facts and figures provide a foundation for some key questions: Where is all of this money going, how much is it helping, and how can better outcomes be achieved?
One reason why the donations described above do not necessarily achieve meaningful progress on these important outcomes, such as alleviating the suffering caused by trachoma, is because cognitive biases drive many of our donation decisions. These rationality-inhibiting biases can lead to a misallocation of resources, hereafter referred to as ‘ineffective altruism’. For the purpose of this article, ineffective altruism occurs when an individual has a given preference for a cause — such as curing blindness, protecting animal welfare, or improving access to education — and does not direct their donations to a charity that is highly effective at solving it.
Tremendous social good can be created if the biases that influence ineffective altruism are identified and corrected through better choice architecture where appropriate. In particular, two cognitive biases are important drivers of ineffective altruism: distance bias and the identifiable victim effect.
References
- Giving USA. (2018). Giving USA: The Annual Report on Philanthropy for the Year 2017. Giving USA Foundation. https://lclsonline.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/12/Giving-USA-2018-Annual-Report.pdf
- The World Bank. (2019). GDP (current US$) – Norway. World Bank Open Data. https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/NY.GDP.MKTP.CD?locations=NO
- Fortune Magazine. (2020, August 10). Fortune Global 500 2020. https://fortune.com/global500/
- Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia. (2020, February 7). Global immunization: Worldwide disease incidence. https://www.chop.edu/centers-programs/vaccine-education-center/global-immunization/diseases-and-vaccines-world-view
- World Health Organization. (2020). Trachoma – Epidemiological Situation. https://www.who.int/trachoma/epidemiology/en/
- Jamison, D. T. (2006). Disease control priorities in developing countries (2nd ed.). World Bank Publications.
- Jones, B., Smith, K., & Rock, D. (2018, June 20). 3 biases that hijack performance reviews, and how to address them. Harvard Business Review. https://hbr.org/2018/06/3-biases-that-hijack-performance-reviews-and-how-to-address-them/
- Singer, P. (1972). Famine, affluence, and morality. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 1(3), 229-243. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315254210-1
- Charities Aid Foundation. (2016, January). Gross Domestic Philanthropy: An international analysis of GDP, tax and giving. https://www.cafonline.org/docs/default-source/about-us-policy-and-campaigns/gross-domestic-philanthropy-feb-2016.pdf
- Hernández-Murillo, R., & Roisman, D. (2005, October 1). The economics of charitable giving: What gives? Federal Reserve Bank of St. Louis. https://www.stlouisfed.org/publications/regional-economist/october-2005/the-economics-of-charitable-giving-what-gives
- Nonprofit Tech for Good. (2018). Global trends in giving report. https://rocklandcce.org/resources/2018-global-trends-in-giving-report
- Kogut, T., Ritov, I., Rubaltelli, E., & Liberman, N. (2018). How far is the suffering? The role of psychological distance and victims’ identifiability in donation decisions. Judgment and Decision Making, 13(5), 458–466. http://journal.sjdm.org/18/18717a/jdm18717a.pdf
- Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2009). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Penguin.
- Thaler, R., & Mullainathan, S. (2000). Behavioral Economics. In International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 5-6). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://www.nber.org/papers/w7948.pdf
- Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207-232. https://doi.org/10.1037/e301722005-001
- Jenni, K., & Loewenstein, G. (1997). Explaining the Identifiable Victim Effect. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14, 235-257. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1023/A:1007740225484
- Ezeh, O. K., Agho, K. E., Dibley, M. J., Hall, J., & Page, A. N. (2014). The impact of water and sanitation on childhood mortality in Nigeria: evidence from demographic and health surveys, 2003-2013. International journal of environmental research and public health, 11(9), 9256–9272. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph110909256
- Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G., & Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and callousness: The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(2), 143-153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.01.005
- Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Murray, D. R. (2015). The evolution of cognitive bias. The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125563.evpsych241
- Aczel, B., Bago, B., Szollosi, A., Foldes, A., & Lukacs, B. (2015). Is it time for studying real-life debiasing? Evaluation of the effectiveness of an analogical intervention technique. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1120. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01120
- Chou, E. Y., & Murnighan, J. K. (2013). Life or death decisions: Framing the call for help. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e57351. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057351
- Jones, C., Hine, D. W., & Marks, A. D. (2016). The future is now: Reducing psychological distance to increase public engagement with climate change. Risk Analysis, 37(2), 331-341. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12601
- Red Cross. (2014). World malaria day: 3.3 billion still at risk. https://www.redcross.ca/blog/2014/4/world-malaria-day-3-3-billion-still-at-risk
About the Author
Julian Hazell
Julian is passionate about understanding human behavior by analyzing the data behind the decisions that individuals make. He is also interested in communicating social science insights to the public, particularly at the intersection of behavioral science, microeconomics, and data science. Before joining The Decision Lab, he was an economics editor at Graphite Publications, a Montreal-based publication for creative and analytical thought. He has written about various economic topics ranging from carbon pricing to the impact of political institutions on economic performance. Julian graduated from McGill University with a Bachelor of Arts in Economics and Management.
About us
We are the leading applied research & innovation consultancy
Our insights are leveraged by the most ambitious organizations
“
I was blown away with their application and translation of behavioral science into practice. They took a very complex ecosystem and created a series of interventions using an innovative mix of the latest research and creative client co-creation. I was so impressed at the final product they created, which was hugely comprehensive despite the large scope of the client being of the world's most far-reaching and best known consumer brands. I'm excited to see what we can create together in the future.
Heather McKee
BEHAVIORAL SCIENTIST
GLOBAL COFFEEHOUSE CHAIN PROJECT
OUR CLIENT SUCCESS
$0M
Annual Revenue Increase
By launching a behavioral science practice at the core of the organization, we helped one of the largest insurers in North America realize $30M increase in annual revenue.
0%
Increase in Monthly Users
By redesigning North America's first national digital platform for mental health, we achieved a 52% lift in monthly users and an 83% improvement on clinical assessment.
0%
Reduction In Design Time
By designing a new process and getting buy-in from the C-Suite team, we helped one of the largest smartphone manufacturers in the world reduce software design time by 75%.
0%
Reduction in Client Drop-Off
By implementing targeted nudges based on proactive interventions, we reduced drop-off rates for 450,000 clients belonging to USA's oldest debt consolidation organizations by 46%