Why are we likely to continue with an investment even if it would be rational to give it up?

The Sunk Cost Fallacy

, explained.
Bias

What is the sunk cost fallacy?

The sunk cost fallacy is our tendency to follow through with something that we’ve already invested heavily in (be it time, money, effort, or emotional energy), even when giving up is clearly a better idea.

A cartoon illustrating the sunk cost fallacy. A stick figure stands at the edge of a cliff, throwing coins into the water below, saying, 'I already started, so I might as well keep going...' A fish in the water has a speech bubble saying, 'I’m rich!' while a nearby insect with a speech bubble containing ellipses ('...') observes the situation. The image humorously depicts the irrational decision to continue investing in a failing endeavor due to the amount already invested.

Where this bias occurs

Imagine that you bought a concert ticket a few weeks ago for $50. Beyond the cost, you are super excited. All of your friends will be in attendance and the band playing is one of your favorites. However, on the day of the concert, you feel sick, and it’s raining outside. You know that traffic will be worse because of the rain and that you risk getting sicker by going to the concert. Although it seems that the current drawbacks outweigh the benefits, why are you still likely to choose to go to the concert?

This is known as the sunk cost fallacy. We are likely to continue an endeavor if we have already invested in it, whether it be a monetary investment or the effort we put into the decision. That often means we go against evidence that shows it is no longer the best decision, such as sickness or weather affecting the event.

Related Biases

Individual effects

In economic terms, sunk costs are costs that have already been incurred and cannot be recovered.1 In the previous example, the $50 spent on concert tickets would not be recovered whether or not you attended the concert. Therefore, it should not be a factor in current decision-making. It is irrational to use irrecoverable costs to justify a present decision. If we acted rationally, only future costs and benefits would be taken into account. Regardless of what we have already invested, we will not get it back whether or not we follow through on the decision.

The sunk cost fallacy doesn’t always have to do with money. Imagine you’ve signed up for a new class offered at your university. After the first week, you realize that it doesn’t really match up with your interests and you’d much rather swap it out for something else. Luckily, you can still drop the class. Yet, when you think about it, and factor in that you've already handed in an assignment, you might still opt to stick with it. This is where the sunk cost fallacy kicks in, hand-in-hand with the status quo bias—our preference to keep things the way they are to avoid potential losses.

A cartoon titled 'The Cost of Sunk Cost.' It depicts three stick figures: one on the left saying, 'I did pay $50 for this concert,' another in the center holding a gun and saying, 'Don’t move,' and a third figure on the right, holding a microphone and singing 'Lalala.' The image humorously illustrates the irrational commitment to a bad decision, represented by the person staying at the concert despite an unpleasant situation because of the money already spent.

The sunk cost fallacy means that we are making irrational decisions because we are factoring in influences other than the current alternatives. This fallacy affects many different areas of our lives—such as our relationships, investments, and career paths—leading to suboptimal outcomes.

Relationships

Believe it or not, the sunk cost fallacy can significantly impact our personal relationships. Imagine you're with a long-term partner who no longer makes you happy, but you hesitate to end things because of the time and emotional energy you've already invested into the relationship. Rather than focusing on the potential for a healthier and more fulfilling future, you might find yourself clinging to the past, fearing that leaving your partner would mean all your previous efforts were wasted. 

Investments

The sunk cost fallacy is particularly prevalent in the world of investments. Whether it's in the stock market, real estate, or even a business venture, investors often fall into the trap of holding onto underperforming assets because they’ve already sunk so much money into them. For example, you might continue pouring money into a declining stock, hoping it will bounce back, rather than cutting your losses and reallocating your resources to a more promising opportunity. 

Career Choices

Another area where the sunk cost fallacy can have a profound effect is in career choices. Many of us find ourselves stuck in a job or career path that no longer aligns with our goals or interests but hesitate to make a change because of the time, effort, or education we’ve already put into it. For instance, someone who has spent years working as an engineer might resist switching careers to be a painter even if they’re unhappy because they feel they’ve already gone through years of training (not to mention an expensive degree). However, continuing down a career path that doesn't fulfill you simply because of past investments can lead to long-term dissatisfaction.

Systemic effects

The sunk cost fallacy not only has an impact on small day-to-day decisions like attending a concert. It has also been proven to impact the decisions that institutions, organizations, and governments make.

Education

The sunk cost fallacy can have real implications in the field of education. Schools often invest substantial resources into programs, curricula, or materials that may not yield the desired results. Yet, because of the time and money already invested, educational institutions may be reluctant to discontinue or modify these initiatives. This adherence to sunk costs can hinder the adoption of more effective teaching strategies or innovations that could better serve students.

Organizations

In organizations, the sunk cost fallacy can manifest in a variety of ways, often leading to inefficiencies and missed opportunities. For instance, a company may continue to fund a failing project because of the amount of resources already put into it, even when it's clear that the project is unlikely to succeed. This can happen with everything from product development to marketing campaigns. Employees and managers alike might feel pressured to "make it work" because abandoning the project would mean admitting that the initial investment was a mistake. However, this mindset can prevent organizations from reallocating resources to more promising ventures. 

Government

The sunk cost fallacy is also prevalent in the political sphere, where it can influence policy decisions and spending. Governments may continue to fund large-scale projects that are no longer effective or relevant, simply because of the significant resources already committed. Politicians might fear the political repercussions of abandoning the project, as it could be perceived as a waste of taxpayer money. However, continuing to invest in ineffective policies or projects because of sunk costs can lead to further inefficiencies and missed opportunities for better solutions. 

The Concorde fallacy is a famous example of sunk costs impacting large-scale decisions. In 1956, the Supersonic Transport Aircraft Committee met to discuss building a supersonic airplane, the Concorde.2 French and British engine manufacturers and their governments were involved in the project, which was estimated to cost almost 100 million dollars.3 Long before the project was over, it was clear that there were increasing costs and that the financial gains of the plane, once in use, would not offset them.4 However, the project continued. The manufacturers and governments followed through on the project because they had already made significant financial investments and dedicated a lot of time to the project.4 Ultimately, this led to millions of dollars wasted, and the Concorde operated for less than 30 years.2

A cartoon showing a stick figure on the left side saying, 'Vive le Concorde!' while pointing towards a red Concorde airplane flying to the right. The airplane has 'Air France' written on its side. The image references the Concorde, a supersonic passenger airliner, symbolizing admiration despite its eventual retirement due to high operating costs.

If governments and large companies like those involved in the Concorde project are susceptible to cognitive fallacies such as sunk cost, it is easy to see that significant amounts of money, time, and effort are wasted. This is because the sunk costs will never be recovered regardless of whether the project is continued or abandoned. Since governments sometimes use taxpayers' money for projects, their adherence to the sunk cost fallacy can negatively affect us all.

Why it happens

The sunk cost fallacy occurs because we are not purely rational decision-makers, and we are often influenced by our emotions. When we have previously invested in a choice, we will likely feel guilty or regretful if we do not follow through. The sunk cost fallacy is associated with commitment bias, where we continue to support our past decisions despite new evidence suggesting that it isn’t the best course of action.

We fail to consider that whatever time, effort, or money we have already expended will not be recovered. We end up making decisions based on past costs instead of present and future costs and benefits, which are the only ones that rationally make a difference.

The sunk cost fallacy may, in part, occur due to loss aversion, which describes that the impact of losses feels much worse to us than the impact of gains. We are more likely to avoid losses than seek out gains. We may feel that our past investment will be ‘lost’ if we don’t follow through on the decision, and make our choice based on loss aversion rather than consider the benefits gained if we do not continue our commitment.

One of the reasons not following through on a decision leads to a feeling of loss is because the situation gets framed together, instead of in stages. If we don’t follow through on a decision, the narrative is one of failure, even if the subsequent choice not to continue is actually in our best interest. Even if costs are higher when we decide to follow through on a decision, such as going to the concert despite the rain and sickness, we can still frame the narrative as an overall success. Otherwise, the story would be that we wasted $50, not that we made an intelligent decision for our health and well-being.

Why it is important

As observed through these various examples, the sunk cost fallacy impacts many aspects of our daily lives, as well as bigger decisions that have long-term effects. The sunk cost fallacy means that we are making irrational decisions that lead to suboptimal outcomes. We are focused on our past investments instead of present and future costs and benefits, meaning that we commit to decisions that are no longer in our best interests.

The sunk cost fallacy is a vicious cycle because we continue to pour resources into endeavors that we have already invested in. The more we invest, the more we feel committed to continuing the project, and the more money, time, and effort we are likely to put in. 

How to avoid it

While it is difficult to overcome cognitive fallacies, if we are aware of the sunk cost fallacy, we can try to focus on current and future costs and benefits instead of past commitments. We should concentrate on concrete actions instead of the feeling of wastefulness or guilt that accompanies dropping an earlier commitment. As studies have shown, when we are deterred from making decisions based on our emotions, the effects of the sunk cost fallacy are reduced.5

A cartoon depicting a stick figure sitting cross-legged in a meditative pose with a content expression. The word 'Debiasing' is written at the top, and the figure has a speech bubble saying 'YAS.' The image humorously conveys the concept of debiasing as a calm and affirming process.

However, it is difficult for us to ignore our emotions as they are powerful influences on our decisions. Instead, we can turn to technology to help us make decisions. Information technology systems make rational choices and are not impacted by the chain of decisions that came before.6

Here are some additional strategies to try out to help you overcome the sunk cost fallacy once and for all.

Setting Clear Goals

One effective way to avoid the sunk cost fallacy is by setting clear, well-defined goals from the outset. When you have a clear vision of what you want to achieve, it becomes easier to evaluate whether continuing with a current course of action is helping you move toward that goal. By focusing on your objectives rather than past investments, you can make decisions that are more aligned with your long-term success.

Making Data-Driven Decisions

Another strategy to counter the sunk cost fallacy is to rely on data-driven decisions. When you base your choices on objective statistics rather than emotions or past investments, you’re more likely to make rational decisions that serve your best interests. By consistently reviewing relevant outcomes, you can more easily recognize when it's time to pivot or abandon a failing course of action.

Using Decision Matrices

A decision matrix can be a powerful tool to help you avoid the sunk cost fallacy.14 By systematically evaluating various options within a structured framework, this approach allows you to weigh the potential future benefits against the past costs, helping you to move past any irrational attachment to sunk costs and focus on what will truly benefit you in the long run.

FAQ

Is the sunk cost fallacy always a bad thing?

Well, a fallacy, by definition, is something “bad”—in the sense that you’re making a decision that leads to sub-optimal outcomes. That said, biases, heuristics, and fallacies exist for a reason. In the case of the sunk cost fallacy, we can think of it as the dark side of our brain’s ability to commit to long-term goals. One of the things that makes humans unique is our ability to plan, collaborate on, and execute goals that might have extremely long time horizons (sometimes even outlasting us). One possible explanation of the sunk cost fallacy is that while this “commitment superpower“ has helped us to achieve a lot compared to other species—but it comes with an irrational attachment to finishing what we’ve already started.

Do we ever gain anything by indulging in the sunk cost fallacy?

It depends on your mindset. If you have a growth mindset, then you could think of every over-investment you’ve made as a learning experience. Yes, maybe that relationship should have ended a year before it did… but did you learn something in the meantime? The sunk cost fallacy might keep us learning and experiencing more instead of abandoning a course of action as soon as it seems unfavorable. Despite the crappy circumstances, it may be worth going to that concert after all.

What is the opposite of a sunk cost?

The opposite of a sunk cost is a prospective cost.13 While a sunk cost refers to money, time, or resources that have already been invested and cannot be recovered, a prospective cost is one that still hasn’t happened yet—providing us with some hope for change. Think of a concert that you are considering purchasing tickets for, a yoga class you might want to sign up for, or even a person you’ve gone on a date with and now might want to make things official with! Unlike sunk costs, we should carefully consider these prospective costs when making these decisions… before it’s too late to change our minds. By focusing on these future expenses, we can better evaluate the potential benefits and drawbacks of continuing or changing a course of action. This forward-looking approach helps avoid the trap of throwing good money after bad.

How it all started

Behavioral scientists and economists are constantly trying to understand why we make irrational decisions. Richard Thaler, a pioneer of behavioral science, first introduced the sunk cost fallacy, suggesting that “paying for the right to use a good or service will increase the rate at which the good will be utilized”.7

Two influential psychologists, Hal Arkes and Catherine Blumer, wanted to examine the sunk cost effect in practice to expand Thaler’s definition beyond money. They defined the fallacy as “a greater tendency to continue an endeavor once an investment in money, effort, or time has been made”.8

Arkes and Blumer conducted several experiments to show that the sunk cost fallacy influences people in their decision-making. The first experiment was a questionnaire study, where participants were asked to imagine that they had spent $100 on a ski trip to Michigan and later $50 on a ski trip to Wisconsin without realizing that the tickets were for the same weekend. They were told to assume that they would enjoy the Wisconsin trip more. Participants were then asked which of the ski trips they would go on if it was too late to return either ticket.

54% of participants said they would go on the Michigan trip, although the rational choice would be to go on the ski trip which would be most enjoyable because costs are lost either way. Arkes and Blumer concluded that over half of the participants chose Michigan because they had made a greater initial investment, providing evidence for the sunk cost fallacy.

Arkes and Blumer wanted to ensure that the sunk cost fallacy was still apparent in a real-life situation rather than in a hypothetical questionnaire. The researchers decided to provide discounted seasonal tickets at a theater to see if the money spent on a ticket affected how often people attended the shows.

People either paid the normal price ($15), were given a $2 discount, or were given a $7 discount, but only after indicating that they wanted to buy a seasonal ticket (this showed that they were willing to pay the original price). Arkes and Blumer then recorded how many shows each individual went to and found that individuals in the no-discount group went to an average of 4.11 shows, compared to 3.32 shows for the individuals in the $2 discount group and 3.29 shows for the individuals in the $7 discount group. Arkes and Blumer concluded that the reason for the difference between groups was that the no-discount group had the greatest sunk costs and therefore continued to invest their time into going to the theatre.8

How it affects product

In creating and selling products, deciding whether to continue or back out of a project is challenging, especially when time, effort, and money are poured into a specific venture. By the rules of the sunk cost fallacy, one may prefer to continue with a project, knowing that it may not be the most profitable or successful if the alternative is giving it up. 

For a company to create valuable and successful products, it is important to be aware of the sunk cost fallacy and how it can impede productivity. Managers and product designers must be able to recognize when a project isn’t going as planned, and when they must make the decision to discontinue ideas when appropriate, regardless of what they have already invested. 

The sunk cost fallacy and AI

Reducing the impact of the sunk cost fallacy may be facilitated by the use of AI machine learning tools. Artificial intelligence tools can help us to determine when a project is worth giving up and when it is worth pursuing. Not influenced by emotion, AI can accurately predict what strategies will likely work and what should be abandoned. In an article by Merlin Stone and colleagues, researchers examined decision-making in business and how this can be supported by AI.13 AI is being used to identify risks in operational marketing that can slip through the cracks of human judgment. 

Example 1 – Educational Choices

Investing in education requires a lot of effort, time, and money, often before the education even begins. The costs of education can therefore be thought of as sunk costs.

Since education can be very expensive, with postsecondary institutions in the U.S. being a $584 billion dollar industry in 2016-20179, psychologist Dr. Martin Coleman wanted to see whether the sunk cost fallacy comes into play with regard to decisions to continue education.10

Colemans’ participants were all told that they had been offered a job that required them to obtain a qualification in communication skills that would cost around $100. They were told that their employers said all the courses were the same and they could take any of them. Participants were informed of one course that had a 75% pass rate, which was at a discounted rate “for today only.” Some participants had the discounted rate set at $50, others $100, and others $150, so as not to miss the deal the participants were notified that they were already signed up 

Participants were then told that their friend had found an identical, free course that had an 85% success rate so they had signed them up. Participants were then asked the following three questions:

Do you go to the college classes that you spent an under/on/over budget amount on? Do you go to the free classes with the better chances of success, or do you attend a few of each?

The rational decision would be for participants to go to the classes with an 85% pass rate, as this decision makes them most likely to succeed. However, Coleman found that participants who had spent over budget ($150) were significantly more likely to remain committed to the initial course even with the lower pass rate, compared to the participants who had spent under budget on the initial course.10

This study suggested that the more money we initially invest in education, the more likely we are to continue with it because of the sunk cost fallacy.

Example 2 – Boring movies

Have you ever realized 30 minutes into watching a movie that you don’t enjoy it but continue to watch it anyway? This is because of the sunk cost fallacy. We continue wasting our time on a boring movie since we have already invested 30 minutes of our time into it.

The likelihood that we will continue watching a boring movie is even greater the younger we are. JoNell Strough, a psychologist interested in how age affects decision-making, along with a team of researchers, investigated this phenomenon.11

Participants in Strough’s study were between 18-27 years old and between 58-91 years old. All participants saw vignettes of two scenarios. In the first scenario, participants were told they had paid $10.95 to watch a movie on pay TV and that five minutes later, they were bored, and it seemed like a bad movie. Participants saw the same scenario in the next vignettes except with the financial investment removed. Participants were then presented with five options for each of the two scenarios: 

  • Stop watching entirely 
  • Watch for five more minutes
  • Watch for ten more minutes
  • Watch for thirty more minutes
  • Watch until the end

Strough found that participants in the 58-91-year-old age group were less likely to fall victim to the sunk cost fallacy, meaning they were less likely to watch the movie until the end or continue watching it for an extended period. Additionally, older participants were more likely to be consistent with their decision for both of the scenarios. Strough concluded that younger people are more likely to be influenced by the sunk cost fallacy and are less consistent with their decisions.11

Summary

What it is

The sunk cost fallacy describes our tendency to continue to pursue an endeavor that we have already committed to in terms of investing money, time, or effort, even if those costs are not recoverable.

Why it happens

The sunk cost fallacy occurs because our emotions often cause us to deviate from rational decisions. Abandoning a project after committing to it and investing resources into it will likely cause negative feelings of guilt and wastefulness. Since we want to avoid negative feelings of loss, we are likely to follow through on a decision that we have invested in, even if it is not in our best interest.

Example 1 - Education choices

Education is a billion-dollar industry in the U.S. and often, we are asked to pay for educational programs in advance. Once we have paid for a particular program, we are unlikely to drop it even if we find a free alternative with a better success rate, this is because we have already invested money into that program.

Example 2 - Boring movies

The sunk cost fallacy also impacts smaller day-to-day decisions, like continuing to watch a movie even if it is boring. We are likely to continue watching a movie if we’ve invested both time and money into it, even though those investments cannot be recovered by continuing to watch the movie. Age seems to impact how much time we are willing to spend continuing to watch a boring movie, with younger people being more susceptible to the sunk cost fallacy.

How to avoid it

Since the sunk cost fallacy is thought to be caused by our desire to avoid negative emotions, we should try to take our emotions out of the equation when making a decision. However, emotions are powerful and hard to ignore. Instead, we may want to use technologies to help us make decisions when it comes to scenarios where the sunk cost fallacy might influence us.

Related TDL articles

Loss Aversion

The sunk cost fallacy is closely related to the bias of loss aversion, which describes how the pain of losing is psychologically more powerful than the pleasure of gaining. Read this TDL article to learn how this bias makes us buy insurance, avoid worthwhile financial risks, and how overcoming it can lead to highly advantageous decisions.

The Decoy Effect

We discussed how framing could influence decision-making by evoking the sunk cost fallacy. In this article, a different method of swaying consumers is discussed ­— the decoy effect. This effect occurs when in addition to two alternatives, a third option is added to influence our perception of the original choices. Read this article to learn more about this trick and how to avoid it.

Sources

  1. Bondarenko, P. (2015, November 23). Sunk cost. Encyclopedia Britannica. https://www.britannica.com/topic/sunk-cost
  2. Blasingame, J. (2011, October 3). Beware of the Concorde fallacy. Forbes. https://www.forbes.com/sites/jimblasingame/2011/09/15/beware-of-the-concorde-fallacy/#5655b8334e22
  3. Amirani, A. (Director). (2003). Concorde: A Love Affair [Timewatch episode]. BBC.
  4. Arkes, H. R., & Ayton, P. (1999). The sunk cost and Concorde effects: Are humans less rational than lower animals? Psychological Bulletin, 125(5), 591-600. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.5.591
  5. Jarmolowicz, D. P., Bickel, W. K., Sofis, M. J., Hatz, L. E., & Mueller, E. T. (2016). Sunk costs, psychological symptomology, and help seeking. SpringerPlus, 5(1), 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40064-016-3402-z
  6. Herrmann, P., Kundisch, D., & Rahman, M. S. (2015). Beating irrationality: Does delegating to IT alleviate the sunk cost effect? SSRN Electronic Journal, 61(4), 831-850. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1941331
  7. Thaler, R. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 1(1), 39-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(80)90051-7
  8. Arkes, H. R., & Ayton, P. (1999). The sunk cost and Concorde effects: Are humans less rational than lower animals? Psychological Bulletin, 125(5), 591-600. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.5.591
  9. National Center For Educational Statistics. (n.d.). Fast facts: Expenditures. Retrieved August 17, 2020, from https://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/
  10. Coleman, M. D. (2010). Sunk cost, emotion, and commitment to education. Current Psychology, 29(4), 346-356. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-010-9094-6
  11. Strough, J., Mehta, C. M., McFall, J. P., & Schuller, K. L. (2008). Are older adults less subject to the sunk-cost fallacy than younger adults? Psychological Science, 19(7), 650-652. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02138.x
  12. Summerfield, C., & Tsetsos, K. (2015). Do humans make good decisions?. Trends in cognitive sciences. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4286584/ 
  13. Roadmunk. (n.d.). Sunk cost. https://roadmunk.com/glossary/sunk-cost/#:~:text=The%20opposite%20of%20a%20sunk,be%20considered%20when%20evaluating%20options
  14. Asana. (n.d.). Sunk cost fallacy: What it is and how to avoid it. https://asana.com/resources/sunk-cost-fallacy

About the Authors

A man in a blue, striped shirt smiles while standing indoors, surrounded by green plants and modern office decor.

Dan Pilat

Dan is a Co-Founder and Managing Director at The Decision Lab. He is a bestselling author of Intention - a book he wrote with Wiley on the mindful application of behavioral science in organizations. Dan has a background in organizational decision making, with a BComm in Decision & Information Systems from McGill University. He has worked on enterprise-level behavioral architecture at TD Securities and BMO Capital Markets, where he advised management on the implementation of systems processing billions of dollars per week. Driven by an appetite for the latest in technology, Dan created a course on business intelligence and lectured at McGill University, and has applied behavioral science to topics such as augmented and virtual reality.

A smiling man stands in an office, wearing a dark blazer and black shirt, with plants and glass-walled rooms in the background.

Dr. Sekoul Krastev

Sekoul is a Co-Founder and Managing Director at The Decision Lab. He is a bestselling author of Intention - a book he wrote with Wiley on the mindful application of behavioral science in organizations. A decision scientist with a PhD in Decision Neuroscience from McGill University, Sekoul's work has been featured in peer-reviewed journals and has been presented at conferences around the world. Sekoul previously advised management on innovation and engagement strategy at The Boston Consulting Group as well as on online media strategy at Google. He has a deep interest in the applications of behavioral science to new technology and has published on these topics in places such as the Huffington Post and Strategy & Business.

About us

We are the leading applied research & innovation consultancy

Our insights are leveraged by the most ambitious organizations

Image

I was blown away with their application and translation of behavioral science into practice. They took a very complex ecosystem and created a series of interventions using an innovative mix of the latest research and creative client co-creation. I was so impressed at the final product they created, which was hugely comprehensive despite the large scope of the client being of the world's most far-reaching and best known consumer brands. I'm excited to see what we can create together in the future.

Heather McKee

BEHAVIORAL SCIENTIST

GLOBAL COFFEEHOUSE CHAIN PROJECT

OUR CLIENT SUCCESS

$0M

Annual Revenue Increase

By launching a behavioral science practice at the core of the organization, we helped one of the largest insurers in North America realize $30M increase in annual revenue.

0%

Increase in Monthly Users

By redesigning North America's first national digital platform for mental health, we achieved a 52% lift in monthly users and an 83% improvement on clinical assessment.

0%

Reduction In Design Time

By designing a new process and getting buy-in from the C-Suite team, we helped one of the largest smartphone manufacturers in the world reduce software design time by 75%.

0%

Reduction in Client Drop-Off

By implementing targeted nudges based on proactive interventions, we reduced drop-off rates for 450,000 clients belonging to USA's oldest debt consolidation organizations by 46%

Notes illustration

Eager to learn about how behavioral science can help your organization?